Early Development of Taste and Flavor Preferences and Consequences on Eating Behavior

50 min read /

Early Development of Taste and Flavor Preferences and Consequences on Eating Behavior

Sophie Nicklaus, Camille Schwartz, Sandrine Monnery-Patris, and Sylvie Issanchou

Early childhood is an important period for the development of health. Recent researches using different approaches (experimental and epidemiological) have shown the importance of this early period for metabolic programming. This general concept of “Developmental Origin of Health and Disease” (DOHAD) has been refined in humans to highlight the importance of the first 1,000 days for the development of the health status. Eating behavior also develops during this period, in particular because from conception until the age of 2 years, the mode of feeding drastically evolves, which involves that the child has to learn “how” to eat, but also what to eat, how much food to eat, and in which context. After birth, when foods are orally exposed, infants discover the intrinsic properties of foods, with a variety of tastes, flavors, textures, as well as energy densities. Here, we focus on deciphering the involvement of taste and olfaction in the early establishment of eating behavior. In the frame of the OPALINE French birth cohort (Observatory of Food Preferences in Infants and Children), taste and flavor preferences were studied in children in relation with food acceptance over the first 2 years of life. 

At birth, previous works revealed evidence of differential preferences across tastes, with a preference for the sweet taste and a rejection of the bitter taste, and very few evidence of inborn flavor preference, except when infants had been previously exposed to flavors from the maternal diet. The later evolution of taste and flavor preferences has received little attention. Within the OPALINE cohort, we studied longitudinally in a group of 285 infants the evolution of taste preferences from 3 to 20 months [1] and the evolution of flavor preferences from 8 to 22 months [2]. Taste acceptance trajectories for all primary tastes and for a fat emulsion were modeled. For saltiness, acceptance increased sharply between the ages of 1  3 and 12 months. The trajectories of acceptance were parallel for sweetness, sourness, and the umami taste between 3 and 20 months, with sweetness being preferred. Between 12 and 20 months, the acceptance of all tastes, except bitterness, decreased, and at 20 months, only sweetness was not rejected. The acceptance of bitterness remained stable. For the fat emulsion, acceptance evolved from indifference to rejection. Between 8 and 22 months, unpleasant food odors (trimethylamine, dimethyl disulfide, and butyric acid) lead to avoidance behavior in infants, but pleasant food odors did not elicit specific behavior. Developmental changes were dependent on taste and flavor; thus, a general change in taste or flavor perception is unlikely. 

Taste exposure (i.e., dietary exposure to a variety of foods) might partly explain the taste acceptance trajectories. We showed that a longer breastfeeding duration (likely associated to a longer exposure to the umami tasting glutamate found in breast milk) was associated to a higher preference for the umami taste at 6 months [3]. Moreover, simultaneous to the introduction to complementary foods, taste differential reactivity (within-subject variability across tastes) clearly increased. At the beginning of the complementary feeding period, we showed that a higher preference for sweet, sour, and umami tastes was associated to a higher acceptance of sweet-, sour-, and umami-tasting foods, respectively [4], and, similarly, that rejection of the odor of trimethylamine and of dimethyl disulfide were related to the rejection of fish and sulfurous cheeses, respectively [5]. Further in development, at 20 months, food neophobia was associated to flavor differential reactivity (within subject variability across flavors) but not to taste differential reactivity (within-subject variability across tastes) [6], underlining the importance of olfaction in the development of neophobic reactions. Further studies are necessary to establish a more complete understanding of the programming effect of early dietary taste and flavor exposure on later food choices.

References

  1. Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Szleper E, et al: Infant acceptance of primary tastes and fat emulsion: developmental changes and links with maternal and infant characteristics. Chem Senses 2017;42:593–603.
  2. Wagner S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, et al: Infants’ hedonic responsiveness to food odours: a longitudinal study during and after weaning (8, 12 and 22 months). Flavour 2013;2:19.
  3. Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Laval C, et al: Breastfeeding duration: influence on taste acceptance over the first year of life. Br J Nutr 2013;109:1154–1161.
  4. Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Lange C, et al: The role of taste in food acceptance at the beginning of complementary feeding. Physiol Behav 2011;104:646–652.
  5. Wagner S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, et al: Liking the odour, liking the food. Toddlers’ liking of strongly flavoured foods correlates with liking of their odour. Appetite 2014;81:60–66.
  6. Monnery-Patris S, Wagner S, Rigal N, et al: Smell differential reactivity, but not taste differential reactivity, is related to food neophobia in toddlers. Appetite 2015;95:303–309.

Abstract

 

The first 1,000 days of life constitute an important period for the development of health and eating behavior. While the feeding mode drastically evolves, the child learns “how”, “what,” and “how much” food to eat. When orally exposed, infants discover food properties, with a variety of tastes, flavors, textures, as well as energy densities. Here, we focus on deciphering the involvement of taste and olfaction in the early establishment of eating behavior. In the OPALINE French birth cohort (Observatory of Food Preferences in Infants and Children), taste and flavor preferences were studied in relation to food preferences over the first 2 years. Both taste and flavor preferences evolved during this period. At weaning, a higher preference for sweet, sour, and umami tastes was associated with a higher acceptance of sweet-, sour- and umami-tasting foods, respectively. At 12 months, rejection of the odor of trimethylamine and dimethyl disulfide was related to the rejection of fish and sulfurous cheeses, respectively. Further, at 20 months, food neophobia was associated with odor but not taste differential reactivity, revealing the importance of olfaction in neophobic reactions. Further studies are ongoing to examine the long-term effect of early taste and flavor exposure on food  preferences.
 

Introduction


Early childhood is an important period for the development of health. Recent researches using different approaches (experimental or epidemiological) have shown the importance of this early period for metabolic programming [1]. This general concept of the “Developmental Origin of Health and Disease” (DOHAD) has been refined in humans to highlight the importance of the first 1,000 days for the development of health status [2]. In this context, our specific focus is on understanding how eating behavior is programmed during this period of the first 1,000 days. As a first observation, eating behavior undergoes a strong development during this period, in particular because the mode of feeding drastically evolves from conception until the age of 2 years. This implies that the child has to learn “how” to eat but also “what” to eat, “how much food” to eat, and “in which context” meals take place. After birth, when orally exposed to foods, infants discover their intrinsic properties, with a variety of tastes, flavors, textures, as well as energy densities, and all these aspects become even more salient at the onset of complementary feeding. By the end of the second year, food neophobia, i.e., the refusal of new food, develops [3–5]. We have previously observed that eating behavior established at the end of this period tracks on later into childhood and up to early adulthood [6–9]. Altogether, this calls for a better understanding of the driving factors and the key periods for the establishment of eating behavior during the first 2 years of postnatal life.
Here, we report several works in which we focused on deciphering the involvement of taste and olfaction in the early establishment of eating behavior over the first 2 years of postnatal life. We focused on both senses because they both strongly relate to the oral phase of feeding and because the chemosensory environment evolves strongly during this early period of life [10, 11]. In the frame of the OPALINE French birth cohort (Observatory of Food Preferences in Infants and Children), taste and flavor preferences were studied independently in children, as well as food acceptance and food neophobia over the course of the first 2 years of life.
 

Evolution of Taste and Odor Preferences in Early Life


At birth, previous works revealed evidence of differential preferences across tastes, with a preference for sweet taste, a rejection of bitter taste [12], and very little evidence of inborn flavor preferences (but rather of avoidance of smell of rotten foods [13]), except when infants had been previously exposed to specific flavors from the maternal diet [10, 11]. Thus, inborn taste and flavor preferences are not numerous although marked food preferences exist in adults and even in young children. It is then much needed to understand the factors contributing to the building of food preferences. The evolution of taste and flavor preferences at later stages in early childhood has received little attention. Within the OPALINE cohort, we studied longitudinally in a group of 285 infants the evolution of taste preferences from 3 to 20 months [14, 15] and the evolution of flavor preferences from 8 to 22 months [16].
Acceptance of each taste (sweet, salty, bitter, sour, and umami) and of a fat emulsion relative to water was defined based on ingestion or liking at 3, 6, 12, and 20 months of age [14, 15]. For each taste, 4 bottles were presented (water, tastant, tastant, water). The acceptance of each taste relative to water was defined using proportional variables that are ratios of ingestion or liking evaluated by the experimenter. These data were analyzed with mixed models that accounted for age and subject effects. Taste acceptance trajectories for all primary tastes and for a fat emulsion were modeled. For saltiness, acceptance increased sharply between the ages of 3 and 12 months. The trajectories of acceptance were parallel for sweetness, sourness, and the umami taste between 3 and 20 months, with sweetness being preferred. Between 12 and 20 months, the acceptance of all tastes, except bitterness, decreased, and at 20 months, only sweetness was not rejected. The acceptance of bitterness remained stable. For the fat emulsion, acceptance evolved from indifference to rejection. Moreover, more rejections were reported based on the judgment of the experimenter than of the infant’s liking. Ingestion and liking ratios are rather complementary, and this result highlights that a grimace is not necessarily associated with reduced ingestion.
Similarly, acceptance of each food odor was defined using proportional variables based on behavioral analysis [16]. Four control stimuli and 8 odors (4 rated by adults as a priori pleasant and 4 as a priori unpleasant) were presented in bottles to infants at 8, 12, and 22 months of age. The infant’s exploratory behavior towards odorized and control bottles was measured in terms of mouthing defined as a direct contact with perioral and/or perinasal areas. For each odorized bottle, durations of mouthing were calculated relative to the control bottles. In this age range, shorter durations of mouthing were found for unpleasantly scented bottles (trimethylamine, dimethyl disulfide, and butyric acid) than pleasantly scented bottles. So, between 8 and 22 months, unpleasant food odors lead to avoidance behavior in infants, but pleasant food odors did not elicit specific behaviors. In these studies, we noted that developmental changes were dependent on taste and odor. We will now examine to which extent these developmental changes also depend on the taste and flavor experiences the infant receives during this period.
 

Effect of Early Taste and Flavor Experiences on Taste and Food Preferences


Several pieces of work have shown that the flavors of the foods consumed by the mother may be transferred into the amniotic fluid and the milk [17–19], but this phenomenon is variable and not yet fully known [10, 11].
Nevertheless, the exposure to specific flavors in utero is associated to a higher attraction for the specific food odor at birth [17], to a higher acceptance of the specific food the infant was exposed to in utero when this food is offered at the beginning of complementary feeding [20], as well as to a higher acceptance of a similarly flavored food later in childhood [21].
In a similar fashion, the exposure to flavors in mother’s milk is associated to a higher acceptance of the specific food the infant was exposed to in mother’s milk when this food is offered at the beginning of complementary feeding [20] and to a positive effect of breastfeeding on acceptance of a new food, even in the absence of a specific flavor exposure [22]. This work suggests that the variety of flavor exposure in breast milk may be key and not only the specific exposure to a given flavor. To expand this building knowledge on the effect of early taste and flavor exposure (i.e., dietary exposure to a variety of tastes in foods), in the OPALINE cohort we wondered about the effect of exposure to specific tastes in the context of milk feeding. It has been described that breast milk contains much more glutamate than formula milk, a compound that is associated with the umami taste. In this context, we observed that infants who were breastfed longer had a higher preference for the umami taste at the age of 6 months [23]. Moreover, concomitant with the introduction of complementary foods which are characterized by a variety of tastes, taste differential reactivity (within-subject variability across tastes; the higher the score, the greater is the difference in reaction of the infant to the tastes) clearly increased [14].
In order to develop our understanding of infant dietary taste exposure, we developed a method to evaluate the taste properties of the diet by describing extensively the intensities of the tastes of all the foods consumed monthly by infants relying on data from “food taste databases” [24]. The application of this method showed that the exposure of French infants to tastes was dominated by exposure to sweet taste over the first year, as illustrated in Figure 1 (in relation to the sweet taste of milk and a number of foods introduced after the initiation of complementary feeding) [25]. We expanded these findings by comparing the dietary exposure to sweet taste and fattiness [26]. They both increased during the first year in relation to the introduction of complementary foods in the child’s diet, but exposure to sweetness increases more rapidly than exposure to fattiness.
However, we have not shown any specific associations between dietary exposure to each taste and acceptance of each taste measured in water, as explained above [27]. The longer-term associations between early exposure to taste and further taste preferences are currently being explored in the OPALINE cohort. Concerning olfactory exposure, we have found that at 8 months of age only, positive correlations were found between liking of some unpleasant odors and early exposure to these odors through the mother’s diet. However, no correlations were found between infants’ liking of the pleasant odors and early exposure to the foods bearing these odors. This study highlights that early exposure to unpleasant food odors may increase subsequent liking (or reduce subsequent dislike) of these food odors at least until the age of 8 months [28].
The originality of the OPALINE study was to evaluate the effect of early odor exposure on the infant through the mother’s spontaneous consumption of a wide spectrum of foods without asking mothers to consume one target food bearing a specific odor quality in sizeable amounts. Such “ecological” research regarding the influence of prenatal and dietary taste and flavor exposure on the establishment of preferences for foods with similar tastes and flavors is still in its beginning. More studies in different cultural contexts were the flavor experience is likely to be contrasted are needed to more completely understand these early imprinting  phenomena.

fig 1

Fig. 1. Dietary exposure of infants to the 5 primary tastes over the first year based on dietary data collected in the French OPALINE cohort (adapted from Schwartz et al. [25] with permission).

 

The Influence of Taste and Odor Preferences on Infants’ Eating Behavior

To further explore the question of the association between taste or flavor preferences and food preferences, we further explored data from the OPALINE cohort. This was evaluated separately for taste and flavor. Concerning taste, we characterized the taste intensities of all foods introduced at the beginning of the complementary feeding period. In parallel, mothers were asked to report their infant’s acceptance of these first foods. We applied classification methods to the taste profiles of the foods, which showed that 15 groups of foods with similar taste profiles had been given to children (e.g., salty foods, sour foods, and sour and sweet fruits) [29]. Then, we compared the aver- age acceptance of all food groups and showed that the acceptance of new foods varied according to their taste profiles. More specifically, we showed that the acceptance of salty vegetables was higher than the acceptance of bitter vegetables, or of sweet and bitter vegetables [29]. Furthermore, we showed that a higher preference for sweet, sour, and umami tastes was associated to a higher accep- tance of some sweet-, sour- and umami-tasting foods, respectively [29]. This supports the hypothesis that the preference for some foods was partly related to the specific preference for their taste properties. Finally, an exploration of the factors related to vegetable acceptance along the first 2 years showed that a high- er bitterness acceptance was associated with a higher acceptance of vegetables at the age of 1 year [30].
Concerning flavors, we similarly explored the acceptance of foods bearing specific odors and evaluated whether their acceptance was related to the acceptance of the corresponding odors (presented alone in scented bottles). We found that at 12 months rejection of the odor of trimethylamine and dimethyl disulfide was related to the rejection of fish and sulfurous cheeses between 12 and 15 months, respectively [31]. Therefore, in the case of olfaction, the flavor-food acceptance associations concerned  foods  with  strong,  unpleasant  flavors  at  12 months only, suggesting that the olfactory system acts as an “alarm” system during this period of food transition.
Eating behavior in children is characterized by its evolution as far as food neophobia is concerned. By the end of the second year, neophobic reactions start happening and can also be designated as picky eating behavior. This developmental phase concerns most children, but we were interested in evaluating whether neophobic reactions could be related to differences in taste and smell acceptance. To evaluate this aspect, we considered taste differential reactivity by computing within-subject variability across tastes as well as flavor differential reactivity by computing within-subject variability across flavors. We found that at 20 months, food neophobia was associated to flavor differential reactivity but not to taste differential reactivity [32], stressing the importance of olfaction in the development of neophobic reactions.
Finally, we have developed a working model of chemosensory, experiential, and environmental factors likely to influence food likes at the age of 2 years (Fig. 2). In this model, we included parental feeding practices and feeding style in the environmental and experiential factors likely to influence likes. The evaluation of this model showed that most of the factors hypothesized to influence liking for vegetables at the age of 2 years had a significant influence, but not the variety of early flavor exposure (in utero    and in mother’s milk), when all factors are taken into account in the same model [30].

fig 2

Fig. 2. Working model of the various early factors which are likely to influence the development of food likes at the age of 2 years. Among the putative factors, it was previously shown that exposure to food-related flavors (in utero or in breast milk) may impact acceptance of new foods at the time of onset of complementary feeding (CF), because the amniotic fluid or breast milk may be flavored by the flavors from the mother’s diet. The reactivity of the infant to sensory stimuli, in particular food-related tastes and odors, is also likely to modulate his/her acceptance of new foods. At the onset of CF, repeated exposure to a given food was shown to strongly impact its acceptance; moreover, exposure to a variety of foods within a given category impacts the acceptance of other new foods from this category. Tracking of eating habits was previously shown but did not necessarily cover well the very early period. Finally, beyond the impact of the family feeding practices, it was previously shown that parental feeding style is also likely to impact food likes in young children.

Conclusions
 

It was shown that children are born with the ability to taste, smell, and discriminate foods, and also to learn to like a new food and its sensory properties. However, marked reactions to taste and olfactory inputs may hinder the learning processes and may be associated with the rejection of specific foods with pronounced tastes and/or flavors. Through the effect of eating experience, taste and olfactory cues acquire a biological significance in relation to the foods they are associated with. Observation of the development of taste and flavor preferences in children shows that learning abilities are high between the onset of complementary feeding and the age of 2 years. This period clearly appears as a window of opportunity to introduce foods from the family diet, particularly vegetables. Learning processes also happen in interaction with parental feeding style and practices, which have to be taken into account to fully understand the development of children’s eating behavior. The research on the early development of food preferences in relation to taste and flavor exposure is still in its infancy. Much more has to be learned: the type of exposure that is more likely to alter food preferences further, the most important periods for this chemosensory exposure, and the conditions under which they may resist to the influence of other factors which are also likely to influence the development of eating behavior and food preferences.

 

References

  • 1 Gillman MW: The first months of life: a critical period for development of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 2008;87:1587–1589.

  • 2 Bhutta ZA, Das JK, Rizvi A, et al: Evidence- based interventions for improvement of maternal and child nutrition: what can be done and at what cost? Lancet 2013;382:452–477.

  • 3 Cashdan E: A sensitive period for learning about food. Hum Nat 1994;5:279–291.

  • 4 Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JCG: Food neophobia and “picky/fussy” eating in children: a review. Appetite 2008;50: 181–193.

  • 5 Nicklaus S: Development of food variety in children. Appetite 2009;52:253–255.

  • 6 Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S: A prospective study of food preferences in childhood. Food Qual Prefer 2004;15:805– 818.

  • 7 Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S: A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite 2005;44:289–297.

  • 8 Schwartz C, Scholtens P, Lalanne A, et al: Development of healthy eating habits early in life: review of recent evidence and selected guidelines.Appetite 2011;57:796–807.

  • 9 Nicklaus S, Remy E: Early origins of overeating: tracking between early food habits and later eating patterns. Curr Obes Rep 2013;2: 179–184.

  • 10 Schaal B: How amniotic fluid shapes early odor-guided responses to colostrum and milk (and more); in Etiévant P, Guichard E, Salles C, Voilley A (eds): Flavor: From Food to Be- haviors, Wellbeing and Health. Cambridge, Elsevier, 2016, chapt 2, pp 23–54.

  • 11 Nicklaus S: Relationships between early flavor exposure, and food acceptability and neophobia; in Etiévant P, Guichard E, Salles C, Voilley A (eds): Flavor: From Food to Behaviors, Wellbeing and Health. Cambridge, Elsevier, 2016, chapt 13, pp 293–311.

  • 12 Berridge KC: Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neurosci Bio behav Rev 2000;24:173–198.

  • 13 Soussignan R, Schaal B, Marlier L, Jiang T: Facial and autonomic responses to biological and artificial olfactory stimuli in human neonates: re-examining early hedonic discrimination of odors. Physiol Behav 1997;62:745– 758.

  • 14 Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Szleper E, et al: Infant acceptance of primary tastes and fat emulsion: developmental changes and links with maternal and infant characteristics. ChemSenses 2017;42:593–603.

  • 15 Schwartz C, Issanchou S, Nicklaus S: Developmental changes in the acceptance of the five basic tastes in the first year of life. Br J Nutr 2009;102:1375–1385.

  • 16 Wagner S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, et al: Infants’ hedonic responsiveness to food odours: a longitudinal study during and after weaning (8, 12, and 22 months). Flavour 2013;2:19.

  • 17 Schaal B, Marlier L, Soussignan R: Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother’s diet. Chem Senses 2000;25:729–737.

  • 18 Mennella JA: Mother’s milk: a medium for early flavor experiences. J Hum Lact 1995;11: 39–45.

  • 19 Hausner H, Bredie WLP, Mølgaard C, et al: Differential transfer of dietary flavour compounds into human breast milk. Physiol Behav 2008;95:118–124.

  • 20 Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK: The early development of human flavor preferences; in Capaldi ED (ed): Why We Eat What We Eat. The Psychology of Eating. Washington, American Psychological Association, 2001, pp 83–112.

  • 21 Hepper PG, Wells DL, Dornan JC, Lynch C: Long-term flavor recognition in humans with prenatal garlic experience. Dev Psychobiol 2013;55:568–574.

  • 22 Hausner H, Nicklaus S, Issanchou S, et al: Breastfeeding facilitates acceptance of a novel dietary flavour compound. Clin Nutr 2010; 29:141–148.

  • 23 Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Laval C, et al: Breastfeeding duration: influence on taste acceptance over the first year of life. Br J Nutr 2013;109:1154–1161.

  • 24 Martin C, Visalli M, Lange C, et al: Creation of a food taste database using an in-home “taste” profile method. Food Qual Prefer 2014;36:70–80.

  • 25 Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Boggio V, et al: To which tastes are infants exposed during the first year of life? Arch Pediatr 2010;17:1026– 1034.

  • 26 Yuan WL, Lange C, Schwartz C, et al: Infant dietary exposures to sweetness and fattiness increase during the first year of life and are associated with feeding practices. J Nutr 2016;146:2334–2342.

  • 27 Schwartz C: Dynamic of taste preferences in infants: effect of food experiences and impact on food acceptance (in French); Doctorat en Science de l’Alimentation. Dijon, University of Burgundy, 2009.

  • 28 Wagner S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, et al: Weanling infants prefer the odors of green vegetables, cheese and fish when their mothers consumed these foods during pregnancy and/or lactation; in revision.

  • 29 Schwartz C, Chabanet C, Lange C, et al: The role of taste in food acceptance at the beginning of complementary feeding. Physiol Behav 2011;104:646–652.

  • 30 Nicklaus S, Chabanet C, Lange C, et al: Unravelling some early determinants of liking for vegetables at 2 years: a longitudinal study from pregnancy to 2 years. 22nd Annual Meeting of the Society for the Study of Ingestive Behavior. Seattle, 2014.

  • 31 Wagner S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, et al: Liking the odour, liking the food. Toddlers’ liking of strongly flavoured foods correlates with liking of their odour. Appetite 2014;81: 60–66.

  • 32 Monnery-Patris S, Wagner S, Rigal N, et al: Smell differential reactivity, but not taste differential reactivity, is related to food neophobia in toddlers. Appetite 2015;95:303–309.